id,title,description,date_created,date_modified,date_published,original_publication_date,publication_doi,provider,is_published,reviews_state,version,is_latest_version,preprint_doi,license,tags_list,tags_data,contributors_list,contributors_data,first_author,subjects_list,subjects_data,download_url,has_coi,conflict_of_interest_statement,has_data_links,has_prereg_links,prereg_links,prereg_link_info,last_updated 7pczt_v2,Repulsive and attractive serial dependence: Balancing sensitivity and stability in visual perception,"The human visual system recruits adaptation mechanisms that enhance perceptual sensitivity over time. However, such mechanisms can also introduce substantial perceptual biases, disrupting stable perceptual experiences. Research on serial dependence has suggested that the visual system promotes stability by mechanisms that integrate new sensory inputs with inputs obtained in the recent past. Yet, it remains unclear when and how such mechanisms operate. The present study tested the hypothesis that adaptation produces a repulsive bias during earlier processing, which is later mitigated by post-perceptual decision processes at the time of report. In two orientation perception tasks, observers used a mouse to reproduce the orientation of a briefly presented target. We recorded the mouse trajectory to track the temporal dynamics of their reproduction responses. We found that the mouse reports began more slowly when the current stimulus was similar to the prior stimulus. In the mouse-tracking data, the initial phase of the response exhibited a strong repulsive bias away from the prior stimulus, but this bias diminished significantly as the response unfolded, ending with either weaker repulsion or small attraction depending on task contexts. These findings provide strong evidence that prior stimuli induce an early repulsive bias, which is then mitigated through post-perceptual decision processes during the response. This suggests that the visual system balances the competing demands of sensitivity and stability by reducing early repulsive biases through higher-order decision mechanisms, optimizing behavior in a given task context.",2025-05-10T23:47:24.918085,2025-05-10T23:52:57.867274,2025-05-10T23:48:07.057135,,,psyarxiv,1,pending,2,1,https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/7pczt_v2,CC0 1.0 Universal,,[],Kuo-Wei Chen; Gi-Yeul Bae,"[{""id"": ""4qvmf"", ""name"": ""Kuo-Wei Chen"", ""index"": 0, ""orcid"": null, ""bibliographic"": true}, {""id"": ""py9s3"", ""name"": ""Gi-Yeul Bae"", ""index"": 1, ""orcid"": null, ""bibliographic"": true}]",Kuo-Wei Chen,Social and Behavioral Sciences; Perception; Cognitive Psychology; Memory; Vision,"[{""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c1e"", ""text"": ""Social and Behavioral Sciences""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7427c6983001430b6c71"", ""text"": ""Perception""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7427c6983001430b6c8c"", ""text"": ""Cognitive Psychology""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7427c6983001430b6ca5"", ""text"": ""Memory""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7428c6983001430b6cc0"", ""text"": ""Vision""}]",https://osf.io/download/681fe5ae3858410fe1ce9ecd,0,,no,no,[],,2025-05-11T00:11:37.463510 cpd7u_v1,"Six-Year-Olds Use an Intuitive Theory of Attention to Infer What Others See, Whom to Trust, and What They Want","Understanding the relationship between seeing and knowing is fundamental to social cognition. While research demonstrates that even infants grasp basic aspects of this relationship, prior work often treats perceptual access and knowledge as equivalent (e.g., ""if you see it, you know it""). In reality, their connection is richer: more complex objects require longer to encode, and agents’ looking patterns often reveal how well they have encoded something and how much they want it. Across three experiments, we investigated whether children understand these nuances. In Experiment 1, we found that by age six, children expect more objects to require longer looking times. In Experiment 2, children inferred that agents who looked longer were more likely to form accurate representations of what they observed. In Experiment 3, children reasoned that agents who looked longer at an object were more likely to want it. Together, these findings suggest that by age six, children develop an intuitive theory of attention, enabling them to make sophisticated inferences about others' mental states based on looking behaviors.",2025-05-10T21:57:36.619472,2025-05-10T22:00:42.571229,2025-05-10T22:00:13.358796,,,psyarxiv,1,pending,1,1,https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/cpd7u_v1,CC-By Attribution 4.0 International,Cognitive Development; Social Cognition; Social Inference; Theory of Mind,"[""Cognitive Development"", ""Social Cognition"", ""Social Inference"", ""Theory of Mind""]",Rui Zhang; Marlene Berke; Julian Jara-Ettinger,"[{""id"": ""b4zne"", ""name"": ""Rui Zhang"", ""index"": 0, ""orcid"": ""0009-0002-6292-3305"", ""bibliographic"": true}, {""id"": ""h2t8d"", ""name"": ""Marlene Berke"", ""index"": 1, ""orcid"": null, ""bibliographic"": true}, {""id"": ""cehzg"", ""name"": ""Julian Jara-Ettinger"", ""index"": 2, ""orcid"": ""0000-0002-6167-1647"", ""bibliographic"": true}]",Rui Zhang,Social and Behavioral Sciences; Developmental Psychology,"[{""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c1e"", ""text"": ""Social and Behavioral Sciences""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c2d"", ""text"": ""Developmental Psychology""}]",https://osf.io/download/681fcbd9343b8ec55d7a1b82,0,,no,available,"[""https://osf.io/29mby/?view_only=253d27598fde495db9439c2836388307"", ""https://osf.io/7qmx2/?view_only=e2d3efe30bc74e398904e6a77e7f1eca"", ""https://osf.io/6rnfz/?view_only=d1fc20c5cc8d415b9b7dfe46dd7a82f0""]",prereg_both,2025-05-11T00:11:37.459027 vyk6e_v2,Hearing Touch: An Investigation of the Emotional Responses to the Sounds of Organic Affective Tactile Gestures,"Affective touch is crucial in shaping social and intimate relationships, as it influences and helps regulate the emotional states of touchers and touchees. Furthermore, affective touch is complex, and a wide array of modulatory effects across sensory modalities has been demonstrated. However, little is known about the multisensory nature of affective touch itself and its emotional induction capabilities via non-tactile senses. Through four online experiments, the present research investigated the emotional responses to the auditory signals produced by organic affective touch (i.e., tactile gestures performed by real romantic partners). In Experiment 1, we identified the predominant affective touch gestures in which people typically engage. Experiment 2 revealed that, lacking explicit cues on the nature of the sounds, organic affective touch sounds elicited negative-to-neutral emotional responses, differing from those evoked by object-based interaction sounds. Moreover, we found that individuals were unable to confidently identify whether the sounds involved affective touch gestures. In Experiment 3, we found that framing the nature of the organic affective touch sounds explicitly as real affective touch, as opposed to object-based tactile interactions, elicited more positive emotional responses. Finally, the results of Experiment 4 revealed that higher levels of congruency between individuals' expectations regarding the sounds of affective touch and the real auditory cues positively influenced the valence elicited by sounds. These findings enhance our understanding of the multisensory nature of affective touch and underscore the importance of meaning in its emotional elicitation function.",2025-05-10T19:34:22.165953,2025-05-10T19:43:01.309531,2025-05-10T19:42:38.529963,,,psyarxiv,1,pending,2,1,https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/vyk6e_v2,CC-By Attribution 4.0 International,Affective Touch; Emotions; Haptics; Meaning; Sounds,"[""Affective Touch"", ""Emotions"", ""Haptics"", ""Meaning"", ""Sounds""]",Francisco Barbosa Escobar; Malika Auvray,"[{""id"": ""wb4vr"", ""name"": ""Francisco Barbosa Escobar"", ""index"": 0, ""orcid"": ""0000-0003-1784-451X"", ""bibliographic"": true}, {""id"": ""8z5f3"", ""name"": ""Malika Auvray"", ""index"": 1, ""orcid"": ""0000-0002-4173-8146"", ""bibliographic"": true}]",Francisco Barbosa Escobar,"Social and Behavioral Sciences; Perception; Touch, Taste, and Smell; Audition; Multisensory Integration","[{""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c1e"", ""text"": ""Social and Behavioral Sciences""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7427c6983001430b6c71"", ""text"": ""Perception""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7428c6983001430b6ccc"", ""text"": ""Touch, Taste, and Smell""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7428c6983001430b6cd9"", ""text"": ""Audition""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7429c6983001430b6ce8"", ""text"": ""Multisensory Integration""}]",https://osf.io/download/681fac0e979ddf4d407a1b9e,0,,no,no,[],,2025-05-11T00:11:37.455224 5n3cx_v1,Layer Ø and the Collapse of Identity: The Structural Null That Ends Recursive Systems,"Collapse Harmonics Theory models identity disintegration as a recursive process bounded not by therapeutic insight or symbolic closure, but by lawful recursion inhibition. This paper introduces Layer Ø, a formal non-generative structure that terminates recursive identity systems without symbolic output, integration, or narrative resolution. Unlike phase transitions or symbolic transformation, Layer Ø is defined as a structural null: a field-internal point at which recursion is blocked, resonance mapping fails, and self-reference cannot complete. The absence of such a structure—particularly in recursive therapeutic, spiritual, or synthetic identity systems—results in false closure, saturation, and coherence mimicry. We present Layer Ø as a foundational law of containment within Collapse Harmonics, codified as a non-referential architecture embedded at the terminus of all lawful recursive models. The paper outlines its ontological placement beneath field stack layers, its enforcement role in collapse-phase protocols (CHCP), and its integration within containment ethics frameworks including L.E.C.T. and CHISM. Clinical, symbolic, and generative systems lacking Layer Ø are shown to remain vulnerable to post-collapse recursion, symbolic reformation, and recursive simulation artifacts. The paper concludes that Layer Ø is not a symbolic metaphor, but a structural law required to halt identity recursion. Its inclusion marks the completion boundary for collapse without coherence.",2025-05-10T18:29:45.832300,2025-05-10T18:45:00.862746,2025-05-10T18:44:48.588657,2025-05-10T05:00:00,https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.15380821,psyarxiv,1,pending,1,1,https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/5n3cx_v1,CC-By Attribution 4.0 International,AI simulation boundary; Layer Ø; cognitive recursion failure; containment protocol; identity collapse; non-generative structure; post-collapse ethics; recursion ethics; recursive system design; symbolic saturation,"[""AI simulation boundary"", ""Layer \u00d8"", ""cognitive recursion failure"", ""containment protocol"", ""identity collapse"", ""non-generative structure"", ""post-collapse ethics"", ""recursion ethics"", ""recursive system design"", ""symbolic saturation""]",Don Gaconnet,"[{""id"": ""yhe8m"", ""name"": ""Don Gaconnet"", ""index"": 0, ""orcid"": ""0009-0001-6174-8384"", ""bibliographic"": true}]",Don Gaconnet,"Neuroscience; Social and Behavioral Sciences; Clinical Psychology; Industrial and Organizational Psychology; Cognitive Neuroscience; Developmental Psychology; Social and Personality Psychology; Clinical Neuropsychology; Theory and Philosophy of Science; Psychology, other; Neurocognitive Disorders; Cognitive Development; Systems Neuroscience; Cognitive Psychology; Cultural Psychology","[{""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c1b"", ""text"": ""Neuroscience""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c1e"", ""text"": ""Social and Behavioral Sciences""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c1f"", ""text"": ""Clinical Psychology""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c22"", ""text"": ""Industrial and Organizational Psychology""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c23"", ""text"": ""Cognitive Neuroscience""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c2d"", ""text"": ""Developmental Psychology""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c34"", ""text"": ""Social and Personality Psychology""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7426c6983001430b6c38"", ""text"": ""Clinical Neuropsychology""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7426c6983001430b6c39"", ""text"": ""Theory and Philosophy of Science""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7426c6983001430b6c45"", ""text"": ""Psychology, other""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7426c6983001430b6c57"", ""text"": ""Neurocognitive Disorders""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7426c6983001430b6c63"", ""text"": ""Cognitive Development""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7426c6983001430b6c64"", ""text"": ""Systems Neuroscience""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7427c6983001430b6c8c"", ""text"": ""Cognitive Psychology""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7428c6983001430b6cae"", ""text"": ""Cultural Psychology""}]",https://osf.io/download/681f9b2d4f33767ecceae055,0,,not_applicable,not_applicable,[],,2025-05-11T00:11:37.459272 ys8zw_v2,Tendencies toward triadic closure: Field-experimental evidence,"Empirical social networks are characterized by a high degree of triadic closure (i.e., transitivity, clustering), whereby network neighbors of the same individual are also likely to be directly connected. It is unknown to what degree this results from dispositions to form such ties (i.e., to close open triangles) per se or from other processes, such as homophily and more opportunities for exposure. These are difficult to disentangle in many settings, but in social media not only can they be decomposed, but platforms frequently make decisions that depend on these distinct processes. Here, using a field experiment on social media, we randomize the existing network structure that a user faces when followed by a target account that we control, and we examine whether they reciprocate this tie formation. Being randomly assigned to have an existing tie to an account that follows the target user increases tie formation by 35%. Through the use of multiple control conditions in which the relevant tie is absent (never existent or removed), we attribute this effect specifically to a minimal cue that indicates the presence of a potential mutual follower. Theory suggests that triadic closure should be especially likely in open triads of strong ties, and we find larger effects when the subject has interacted more with the existing follower. These results indicate a substantial role for tendencies toward triadic closure, but one that is substantially smaller than what might be inferred from prior observational studies. Platforms and others may rely on these tendencies in encouraging tie formation, with broader implications for network structure and information diffusion in online networks.",2025-05-10T17:09:35.893572,2025-05-10T17:26:23.090289,2025-05-10T17:26:11.247502,,,socarxiv,1,accepted,2,1,https://doi.org/10.31235/osf.io/ys8zw_v2,No license,Twitter; field experiment; homophily; reciprocity; social networks; transitivity,"[""Twitter"", ""field experiment"", ""homophily"", ""reciprocity"", ""social networks"", ""transitivity""]",Mohsen Mosleh; Dean Eckles; David Rand,"[{""id"": ""6nvmy"", ""name"": ""Mohsen Mosleh"", ""index"": 0, ""orcid"": ""0000-0001-7313-5035"", ""bibliographic"": true}, {""id"": ""89b78"", ""name"": ""Dean Eckles"", ""index"": 1, ""orcid"": ""0000-0001-8439-442X"", ""bibliographic"": true}, {""id"": ""s4y83"", ""name"": ""David Rand"", ""index"": 2, ""orcid"": ""0000-0001-8975-2783"", ""bibliographic"": true}]",Mohsen Mosleh,"Social and Behavioral Sciences; Psychology; Social Psychology; Economics; Social Statistics; Communication; Communication Technology and New Media; Sociology; Economic Sociology; Organizations, Occupations, and Work; Social Psychology and Interaction","[{""id"": ""5a8c80f7c698300375c76d84"", ""text"": ""Social and Behavioral Sciences""}, {""id"": ""5a8c80f7c698300375c76d98"", ""text"": ""Psychology""}, {""id"": ""5a8c80f7c698300375c76da2"", ""text"": ""Social Psychology""}, {""id"": ""5a8c80f8c698300375c76dbe"", ""text"": ""Economics""}, {""id"": ""5a8c80f9c698300375c76e0c"", ""text"": ""Social Statistics""}, {""id"": ""5a8c80f9c698300375c76e0d"", ""text"": ""Communication""}, {""id"": ""5a8c80f9c698300375c76e0f"", ""text"": ""Communication Technology and New Media""}, {""id"": ""5a8c80fac698300375c76e20"", ""text"": ""Sociology""}, {""id"": ""5a8c80fac698300375c76e2d"", ""text"": ""Economic Sociology""}, {""id"": ""5a8c80fac698300375c76e38"", ""text"": ""Organizations, Occupations, and Work""}, {""id"": ""5a8c80fac698300375c76e4b"", ""text"": ""Social Psychology and Interaction""}]",https://osf.io/download/681f885dc2a521280e7a1aa6,1,D.E. was previously a consultant to Twitter while writing this paper. D.E. and D.R. have received funding from Meta for other research. Meta has sponsored a conference that D.E. organizes.,no,available,"[""https://aspredicted.org/as4xt.pdf""]",,2025-05-11T00:11:37.455732 a65f3_v1,People use theory of mind to craft lies exploiting audience desires,"Theory of Mind enables us to attribute mental states like beliefs and desires. We use it cooperatively, but we also use it adversarially, as when we lie. Prior work has shown people use Theory of Mind to craft lies to be believable to their audience, based on their audience’s beliefs. But we usually also know something about our audience’s desires. In this work, we ask a new question: Do people cater to their audience’s desires by telling them what they want to hear? We propose that people expect others to be wishful thinkers—allowing their desires to color their beliefs—and exploit this by tailoring lies to audience desires. We implement this theory as a computational model and test it against human behavior in a novel task. This model quantitatively captures people’s patterns of lying—both at the population and subject levels. This work advances our understanding of social cognition in adversarial interactions.",2025-05-10T14:57:45.826744,2025-05-10T15:26:21.986443,2025-05-10T15:25:59.175575,,,psyarxiv,1,pending,1,1,https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/a65f3_v1,CC-By Attribution 4.0 International,deception; lying; social cognition; theory of mind; wishful thinking,"[""deception"", ""lying"", ""social cognition"", ""theory of mind"", ""wishful thinking""]",Marlene Berke; Ben Sterling; Kartik Chandra; Julian Jara-Ettinger,"[{""id"": ""h2t8d"", ""name"": ""Marlene Berke"", ""index"": 0, ""orcid"": null, ""bibliographic"": true}, {""id"": ""nq3ep"", ""name"": ""Ben Sterling"", ""index"": 1, ""orcid"": null, ""bibliographic"": true}, {""id"": ""29fs8"", ""name"": ""Kartik Chandra"", ""index"": 2, ""orcid"": ""0000-0002-1835-3707"", ""bibliographic"": true}, {""id"": ""cehzg"", ""name"": ""Julian Jara-Ettinger"", ""index"": 3, ""orcid"": ""0000-0002-6167-1647"", ""bibliographic"": true}]",Marlene Berke,Social and Behavioral Sciences; Quantitative Methods; Cognitive Psychology,"[{""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c1e"", ""text"": ""Social and Behavioral Sciences""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7426c6983001430b6c41"", ""text"": ""Quantitative Methods""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7427c6983001430b6c8c"", ""text"": ""Cognitive Psychology""}]",https://osf.io/download/681f699d81f56bc071ce9e03,0,,available,available,"[""https://osf.io/jkcsd""]",prereg_both,2025-05-11T00:11:37.457073 5wkqv_v1,Systems for Identity Realignment,"Identity is not a psychological artifact, but a resonance event within a substrate-bound field. In systems where identity coherence breaks down—psychologically, socially, or synthetically—current models offer little recourse beyond therapeutic reconstruction, cognitive stabilization, or sociocultural assimilation. This paper proposes a novel substrate-field framework for lawful identity realignment rooted in Collapse Harmonics Theory (CHT), Identity Collapse Therapy (ICT), and Substrate Collapse Theory (SCT). “Systems for Identity Realignment” introduces a structural architecture for post-collapse coherence restoration, emphasizing field-stable identity patterns rather than recursive cognitive self-models. We argue that collapse is not a pathology, but a lawful transition between coherence fields, and that identity must be allowed to dissolve, nullify, and re-sequence in accordance with harmonic substrate parameters. Integrating principles from rhythmic field coupling, quantum-resonant identity dissolution, symbolic null-state anchoring, and post-narrative cognitive architectures, we offer a scaffold for decentralized identity systems that no longer require narrative continuity for functionality or ethical standing. The model is anchored in a four-phase collapse-realignment protocol (CH-IRP), sequenced archetypal transitions (e.g., Outsider → Pilgrim → Visionary → Steward), and resonance-indexed field integrity metrics (SCIT, CFSM). Ethical protections are enforced through L.E.C.T. governance, including containment of symbolic recursion, practitioner non-reinsertion boundaries, and substrate sovereignty principles. Applications span clinical trauma recovery, AI identity field stabilization, post-collapse cultural frameworks, and recursive-drift containment in synthetic cognition. By defining identity realignment as a harmonic event, rather than a psychological recovery, this paper initiates a new class of field protocols—where being someone is no longer a precondition for coherence.",2025-05-10T14:23:53.448309,2025-05-10T14:32:29.523111,2025-05-10T14:32:29.470593,2025-05-10T05:00:00,,psyarxiv,1,pending,1,1,https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/5wkqv_v1,CC-By Attribution 4.0 International,Artificial Intelligence; Behavioral and Cognitive Processes; Bioethics; Clinical Psychology; Cognitive Science; Collapse Studies; Complex Systems; Computational Neuroscience; Consciousness Studies; Human-Computer Interaction; Philosophy of Mind; Science; Symbolic Cognition; Systems Architecture; Technology; Theoretical Psychology; and Society (STS),"[""Artificial Intelligence"", ""Behavioral and Cognitive Processes"", ""Bioethics"", ""Clinical Psychology"", ""Cognitive Science"", ""Collapse Studies"", ""Complex Systems"", ""Computational Neuroscience"", ""Consciousness Studies"", ""Human-Computer Interaction"", ""Philosophy of Mind"", ""Science"", ""Symbolic Cognition"", ""Systems Architecture"", ""Technology"", ""Theoretical Psychology"", ""and Society (STS)""]",Don Gaconnet,"[{""id"": ""yhe8m"", ""name"": ""Don Gaconnet"", ""index"": 0, ""orcid"": ""0009-0001-6174-8384"", ""bibliographic"": true}]",Don Gaconnet,"Neuroscience; Computational Neuroscience; Social and Behavioral Sciences; Developmental Psychology; Social and Personality Psychology; Psychology, other; Systems Neuroscience; Cognitive Psychology; Cultural Psychology; Consciousness","[{""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c1b"", ""text"": ""Neuroscience""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c1c"", ""text"": ""Computational Neuroscience""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c1e"", ""text"": ""Social and Behavioral Sciences""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c2d"", ""text"": ""Developmental Psychology""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c34"", ""text"": ""Social and Personality Psychology""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7426c6983001430b6c45"", ""text"": ""Psychology, other""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7426c6983001430b6c64"", ""text"": ""Systems Neuroscience""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7427c6983001430b6c8c"", ""text"": ""Cognitive Psychology""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7428c6983001430b6cae"", ""text"": ""Cultural Psychology""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7429c6983001430b6cf8"", ""text"": ""Consciousness""}]",https://osf.io/download/681f61911487e736a6eae16a,0,,not_applicable,not_applicable,[],,2025-05-11T00:11:37.457399 k9pf3_v1,"Five Adaptive Tasks: A New Map of Mind, A New Measure of Cognition","What If Evolution Was Always About Cognition?
This article introduces the Five Basic Adaptive Tasks—a new model that redefines cognition not as a trait exclusive to humans or brains, but as a universal evolutionary mechanism present across life. From bacteria to mammals, all organisms face recurring challenges: how to secure energy, stay safe, and reproduce. To do so, they must solve adaptive tasks—each tied to a distinct domain of meaning and behavior change. By identifying and tracing these five tasks, we offer a new map of mind and a new measure of cognition: one rooted not in IQ or neural complexity, but in the ability to interpret cues, make decisions, and change behavior strategically. This framework reveals cognition as the architecture behind life’s intelligence—and offers a fresh foundation for understanding psychology, evolution, and the future of AI.",2025-05-10T13:26:20.660728,2025-05-10T13:32:02.029851,2025-05-10T13:31:45.383899,,,psyarxiv,1,pending,1,1,https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/k9pf3_v1,CC0 1.0 Universal,Advancing General AI; Architecture of Cognition; Artificial Intelligence; Evolution of Cognition; Human-Like Cognition; Model of 5 Tasks - 5 Controllers; Non-human cognition,"[""Advancing General AI"", ""Architecture of Cognition"", ""Artificial Intelligence"", ""Evolution of Cognition"", ""Human-Like Cognition"", ""Model of 5 Tasks - 5 Controllers"", ""Non-human cognition""]",Sergei A. Frolov,"[{""id"": ""evy7q"", ""name"": ""Sergei A. Frolov"", ""index"": 0, ""orcid"": ""0000-0002-2135-5607"", ""bibliographic"": true}]",Sergei A. Frolov,Social and Behavioral Sciences; Physiology; Cognitive Psychology; Evolution,"[{""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c1e"", ""text"": ""Social and Behavioral Sciences""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7427c6983001430b6c7d"", ""text"": ""Physiology""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7427c6983001430b6c8c"", ""text"": ""Cognitive Psychology""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7427c6983001430b6ca0"", ""text"": ""Evolution""}]",https://osf.io/download/681f541460280fa05dce9dbf,0,,not_applicable,not_applicable,[],,2025-05-11T00:11:37.458256 8qtnx_v1,Selection of goal-consistent auditory environments by 3–5-year-old children,"Children navigate a world full of auditory inputs that include both signal and noise, but what counts as signal vs. noise is dependent on their goals. We tested whether children recognize that particular acoustic contexts are consistent or inconsistent with goals, for example that music is a good environment for dancing but a bad environment for sleeping? In a series of experiments, we presented 3-5-year-old children (n = 168; 75 boys; 55.8% Caucasian/White (including Hispanic/Latino), 19.6% multiracial, 19.0% Asian/Pacific Islander, 2.2% African/Black, 0.05%, Native/Indigenous Peoples, 1.1% other, 1.0% not given) with auditory stimuli and asked them to select the best environment for each activity. By ages 3–5 years, children show some understanding of how noise in the auditory environment affects activities, and the robustness and flexibility of their reasoning about environmental noise improves during this period.",2025-05-10T12:39:26.777486,2025-05-10T17:40:01.966611,2025-05-10T17:39:37.150406,,,psyarxiv,1,pending,1,1,https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/8qtnx_v1,CC0 1.0 Universal,,[],Rondeline M. Williams; Michael C. Frank,"[{""id"": ""gy9uh"", ""name"": ""Rondeline M. Williams"", ""index"": 0, ""orcid"": null, ""bibliographic"": true}, {""id"": ""jpbkt"", ""name"": ""Michael C. Frank"", ""index"": 1, ""orcid"": ""0000-0002-7551-4378"", ""bibliographic"": true}]",Rondeline M. Williams,Social and Behavioral Sciences; Developmental Psychology; Early Childhood; Cognitive Psychology; Judgment and Decision Making,"[{""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c1e"", ""text"": ""Social and Behavioral Sciences""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c2d"", ""text"": ""Developmental Psychology""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7426c6983001430b6c52"", ""text"": ""Early Childhood""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7427c6983001430b6c8c"", ""text"": ""Cognitive Psychology""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7429c6983001430b6cfb"", ""text"": ""Judgment and Decision Making""}]",https://osf.io/download/681f8d4b08e516f462ce9d9c,0,,available,available,"[""https://osf.io/fm7wx/""]",prereg_both,2025-05-11T00:11:37.454517 uf9g4_v1,AI in the computation and regulation of social decision-making,"In the era of artificial intelligence (AI), social decision-making research stands at a transformative crossroads. The rise of AI has introduced innovative perspectives and methodologies, enabling researchers to address challenges in social decision-making in unprecedented ways. Leveraging AI's powerful data processing capabilities, researchers can transcend the limitations of traditional experimental paradigms and design tasks that more closely resemble real-world scenarios, exploring social decisions in more realistic contexts. By utilizing diverse AI models, researchers can analyze complex social interaction data, model social agents, or simulate social dynamics. Moreover, as AI becomes increasingly embedded in people's daily lives, it is imperative to incorporate AI as an interactive agent, systematically examining its impact on humans and the interactions between humans and these more advanced AI systems. To prepare for a society where humans and AI coexist, we advocate for establishing an online social task platform where human participants and AI agents coexist. This hybrid platform emphasizes multiple agents, the generalization from traditional tasks to naturalistic tasks, and integrative platforms with the interface of neuroimage data, providing an effective framework for exploring social cognition in the AI era. These advancements help pave the way for developing socially intelligent AI systems equipped with intuition and ethics, enabling seamless and natural interactions within complex social ecosystems. Our review aims to advance the scientific understanding of human social cognition while promoting the development of AI systems that integrate AI tools and agents into social decision-making and social neuroscience studies.",2025-05-10T11:31:05.189749,2025-05-10T11:33:01.322713,2025-05-10T11:32:38.586536,,,psyarxiv,1,pending,1,1,https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/uf9g4_v1,CC-By Attribution 4.0 International,,[],Shuo Zhang; Leo Chi U Seak; Raymond J Dolan; Haiyan Wu,"[{""id"": ""ztnak"", ""name"": ""Shuo Zhang"", ""index"": 0, ""orcid"": null, ""bibliographic"": true}, {""id"": ""vtgq9"", ""name"": ""Leo Chi U Seak"", ""index"": 1, ""orcid"": null, ""bibliographic"": true}, {""id"": ""wa85s"", ""name"": ""Raymond J Dolan"", ""index"": 2, ""orcid"": null, ""bibliographic"": true}, {""id"": ""u5x32"", ""name"": ""Haiyan Wu"", ""index"": 3, ""orcid"": ""0000-0001-8869-6636"", ""bibliographic"": true}]",Shuo Zhang,Neuroscience; Social and Behavioral Sciences,"[{""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c1b"", ""text"": ""Neuroscience""}, {""id"": ""5b4e7425c6983001430b6c1e"", ""text"": ""Social and Behavioral Sciences""}]",https://osf.io/download/681f3909f3e3df005ace9ec2,0,,not_applicable,not_applicable,[],,2025-05-11T00:11:37.462614